It is the cache of ${baseHref}. It is a snapshot of the page. The current page could have changed in the meantime.
Tip: To quickly find your search term on this page, press Ctrl+F or ⌘-F (Mac) and use the find bar.

Examining Oral Reading Fluency Trajectories Among English Language Learners and English Speaking Students | Jimerson | Journal of New Approaches in Educational Research

Examining Oral Reading Fluency Trajectories Among English Language Learners and English Speaking Students
Un estudio de las trayectorias de fluidez lectora oral en estudiantes de inglés como segunda lengua y estudiantes de habla inglesa

Shane R. Jimerson, Sehee Hong, Scott Stage, Michael Gerber

Abstract

Students’ oral reading fluency growth from first through fourth grade was used to predict their achievement on the Stanford Achievement Test (9th ed.; SAT-9 Reading) using a latent growth model. Two conditional variables related to student status were used to determine the effects on reading performance - English language learners (ELLs) with low socioeconomic status and low socioeconomic (SES) status alone. Results revealed that both types of student status variables reliably predicted low performance on initial first grade oral reading fluency, which later predicted fourth grade performance on the SAT-9. However, the reading fluency trajectories of the ELLs and monolingual English students were not significantly different. In addition, when both student status variables and letter naming fluency were used to predict initial oral reading fluency, letter naming fluency dominated the prediction equation, suggesting that an initial pre-reading skill, letter naming fluency, better explained fourth grade performance on the SAT-9 than either ELL with low SES or low SES alone. The discussion focuses on how to better enable these readers and how oral reading fluency progress monitoring can be used to assist school personnel in determining which students need additional instructional assistance.


Keywords

ORAL ENGLISH; ORAL READING FLUENCY; SECOND LANGUAGE LEARNING; FOREIGN LANGUAGE LEARNING; LANGUAGE FLUENCY

Resumen

Los resultados revelaron que ambos tipos de variables sobre la condición del estudiante predecían de manera fiable un bajo desempeño en su fluidez lectora oral en 1o, que después predeciría el desempeño del 4o curso en el SAT-9. Sin embargo, las trayectorias de fluidez de lectura de los estudiantes ELL y de los estudiantes monolingües ingleses no presentaban diferencias significativas. Asimismo, cuando se utilizaron las dos variables sobre la situación del estudiante y la fluidez nombrando letras para predecir la fluidez lectora oral inicial, la fluidez a la hora de nombrar letras dominaba la ecuación de predicción, lo que indicaba que una habilidad inicial de prelectura como es la fluidez a la hora de nombrar las letras explicaba mejor el rendimiento de 4o curso en el SAT-9 que el hecho de ser estudiantes de legua inglesa con bajo estatus socio económico o el bajo estatus socioeconómico por sí sólo. El estudio se centra en cómo capacitar mejor a estos lectores y ver cómo se puede utilizar el seguimiento de su progreso en la lectura oral para ayudar al equipo escolar a determinar qué estudiantes necesitan recibir ayuda adicional en su formación.


Palabras Clave

INGLÉS ORAL, FLUIDEZ LECTORA ORAL, APRENDIZAJE DE SEGUNDA LENGUA, APRENDIZAJE DE LENGUA EXTRANJERA, DOMINIO DEL IDIOMA

References

  • Adams, M. J. (1990). Beginning to read: Thinking and learning about print. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.

  • Arbuckle, J. L. (1996). Full information estimation in the presence of incomplete data. In G.A. Marcoulides & R. E. Schumacker (Eds.), Advanced structural equation modeling: Issues and techniques. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

  • Baker, S. K., & Good, R. (1995). Curriculum-based measurement of English reading with bilingual Hispanic students: A validation study with second-grade students. School Psychology Review, 24(4), 561-578.

  • Baker, S. K., Plasencia-Peinado, J., & Lezcano-Lytle, V. (1998). The use of curriculum-based measurement with language-minority students. In M.R. Shinn (Ed.), Advanced Applications of Curriculum-Based Measurement (pp. 175-213). New York: The Guilford Press.

  • Bentler, P. M., & Bonett, D. G. (1980). Significance tests and goodness of fit in the analysis of covariance structures. Psychological Bulletin, 88, 588-606. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.88.3.588

  • Bentz, J., & Pavri, S. (2001). Curriculum-based measurement in assessing bilingual students: A promising new direction. Diagnostique, 25(3), 229-248.

  • Browne, M. W., & Cudeck, R. (1993). Alternative ways of assessing model fit. In K. A. Bollen & J. S. Long (Eds.), Testing structural equation models (pp. 136-162). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.

  • Cauce, A. M., & Domenech-Rodriguez, M. (2002). Latino families: Myths and realities. In J. M. Contreras, K. A. Kerns & A. M. Neal-Barnett (Eds.), Latino Children and Families in the United States: Current Research and Future Directions (pp. 3-25). Westport, CT: Praeger.

  • De Avila, E. A., & Duncan, S. E. (1984). Language Assessment Scales. Monterey, CA: CTB/McGraw-Hill.

  • Deno, S. L. (1985). Curriculum-based measurement: The emerging alternative. Exceptional Children, 52(3), 219-232.

  • Deno, S. L., & Fuchs, L. S. (1987). Developing curriculum-based measurement systems for data-based special education problem solving. Focus on Exceptional Children, 19(8), 1-16.

  • Duncan, S. C., & Duncan, T. E. (1994). Modeling incomplete longitudinal substance use data using latent variable growth curve methodology. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 29, 313-338. doi:10.1207/s15327906mbr2904_1

  • Elliott, J., Lee, S. W., & Tollefson, N. (2001). A reliability and validity study of the Dynamic Indicators of Basic Early Literacy Skills-Modified. School Psychology Review, 30, 33-49.

  • Francis, D. J., Shaywitz, S. E., Stuebing, K. K., & Fletcher, J. M. (1996). Developmental lag versus deficit models of reading disability: A longitudinal, individual growth curves analysis. Journal of Educational Psychology, 88, 1-37. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.88.1.3

  • Fuchs, L. S., & Deno, S. L. (1994). Must instructionally useful performance assessment be based in the curriculum?. Exceptional Children, 61, 15-24.

  • Fuchs, L. S., & Deno, S. L. (1991). Paradigmatic distinctions between instructionally relevant measurement models. Exceptional Children, 57(6), 488-499.

  • Fuchs, L. S., Fuchs, D., Hamlett, C. L., Walz, L. & Germann, G. (1993). Formative evaluation of academic progress: How much growth can we expect?. School Psychology Review, 22, 27-48.

  • Good, R. H., & Jefferson, G. (1998). Contemporary perspectives on curriculum-based measurement validity. In M. R. Shinn (Ed.), Advanced application of Curriculum-Based Measurement (pp. 61-88). New York: Guilford Press.

  • Greenberg, E., Macias, R. E., Rhodes, D., & Chan, T. (2001). English literacy and language minorities in the United States. Education Statistics Quarterly, 3(4), 73-75.

  • Habendank-Stewart, L., & Kaminski, R. (2002). Best practices in curriculum-based evaluation. In A. Thomas & J. Grimes (Eds.), Best practices in school psychology IV. Bethesda, MD: The National Association of School Psychologists.

  • Harcourt Brace & Company (1997a). Stanford Achievement Test Series – Ninth Edition: Spring norms book. San Antonio, TX: Harcourt Brace & Company.

  • Harcourt Brace & Company (1997b). Stanford Achievement Test Series – Ninth Edition: Technical data report. San Antonio, TX: Harcourt Brace & Company.

  • Hasbrouck, J. E., & Tindal, G. (1992). Curriculum-based oral reading fluency norms for students in grades 2 through 5. Teaching Exceptional Children, 24(3), 41-44.

  • Hintze, J. M., Callahan, J. E, Matthews, W. J., Williams, S. A., & Tobin, K. G. (2002). Oral reading fluency and prediction of reading comprehension in African American and Caucasian elementary school children. School Psychology Review, 31(4), 540-553.

  • Howell, K. W., Kurns, S., & Antil, L. (2002). Best practices in curriculum-based evaluation. In A. Thomas & J. Grimes (Eds.), Best practices in school psychology IV (pp.753-770). Bethesda, MD: The National Association of School Psychologists.

  • Hu, L-Z., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling, 6, 1-55. doi:10.1080/10705519909540118

  • Jimerson, S. R. (1997). ORAL-J: The administration and technical manual. Available from S. R. Jimerson, University of California, Santa Barbara, California, 93106-9490.

  • Jimerson, S. R. (2000). ORAL-J: The administration and technical manual. Available from S. R. Jimerson, University of California, Santa Barbara, California, 93106-9490.

  • Klein, J., & Jimerson, S. R. (in press). Examining ethnic, gender, and language bias of oral reading fluency scores among Caucasian and Hispanic students. School Psychology Review.

  • Kranzler, J. H., Miller, M. D., & Jordan, L. (1999). An examination of racial/ethnic and gender bias on Curriculum-Based Measurement of reading. School Psychology Quarterly, 14, 327-342. doi:10.1037/h0089012

  • Marston, D. B. (1987). The effectiveness of special education: A time series analysis of reading performance in regular and special education settings. Journal of Special Education, 21, 13-26.

  • Marston, D., & Magnusson, D. (1985). Implementing curriculum-based measurement in special and regular education settings. Exceptional Children, 52, 176-266. doi:10.1177/002246698802100405

  • McArdle, J., & Epstein, D. (1987). Latent growth curves within developmental structural equation models. Child Develoment, 58, 110-133. doi:10.2307/1130295

  • Meredith, W., & Tisak, J. (1990). Latent curve analysis. Psychometrika, 55, 107-122. doi:10.1007/BF02294746

  • Powell-Smith, K. A., & Bradley-Klug, K. L. (2001). Another look at the “c” in cbm: Does it really matter if curriculum-based measurement reading probes are curriculum-based?. Psychology in the Schools, 38(4), 299-312. doi:10.1002/pits.1020

  • Schafer, J., & Olsen, M. (1998). Multiple imputation for multivariate missing-data problems: A data analyst’s perspective. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 33, 545-571. doi:10.1207/s15327906mbr3304_5

  • Shinn, M. R. (Ed.) (1998). Advanced Applications of Curriculum-Based Measurement. New York: The Guilford Press.

  • Shinn, M. R. (Ed.) (1989). Curriculum-Based Measurement: Assessing Special Children. New York: The Guilford Press.

  • Sibley, D., Biwer, D., & Hesch, A. (April, 2001). Establishing curriculum-based measurement oral reading performance standards to predict success on local and state tests of reading achievement. Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the National Association of School Psychologists, Washington, DC.

  • Stage, S. A. (2001). Program evaluation using hierarchical linear modeling with curriculum-based measurement reading probes. School Psychology Quarterly, 16, 91-112.

  • Stage, S. A., & Jacobsen, D. M. (2001). Predicting student success on a state-mandated performance-based assessment using oral reading fluency. School Psychology Review, 30, 407-419. doi:10.1521/scpq.16.1.91.19159

  • Stage, S. A., Sheppard, J., Davidson, M., & Browning, M. (2001). Prediction of first grader’s growth in oral reading using their kindergarten letter-naming and letter-sound fluency. Journal of School Psychology, 39, 225-237. doi:10.1016/S0022-4405(01)00065-6

  • Stanovich, K. E. (1986). Matthew effects in reading: Some consequences of individual differences in the acquisition of literacy. Reading Research Quarterly, 21, 360-407. doi:10.1598/RRQ.21.4.1

  • US. Department of Education, National Center for Education Statistics (2003). Status and trends in the education of Hispanics (NCES 2003-08). Washington, DC: Author.

  • Tindal, G. (1993). A review of curriculum-based procedures on nine assessment components. In J. J. Kramer (Ed.), Curriculum-based measurement (pp.25-64). Lincoln, NE: Buros Institute of Mental Measurements.

  • Whitehurst, G. J., & Fischel, J. E. (2000). Reading and language impairments in conditions of poverty. In D. V. M. Bishop & L. B. Leonard (Eds.), Speech and language impairments in children: Causes, characteristics, intervention and outcome (pp. 53-71). Hove, England: Psychology Press.  


NAER likes to provide a Spanish translation of all the journal articles to make its contents available to a wider readership. However, this is not the original article but merely a translation. If want to quote this article, please refer back to the original English version also available online, and to the page numbers of the English version

This journal is a Crossref Cited-by Linking member. This list shows the references that citing the article automatically. For more information about the system please visit Crossref site

Select prefered PDF Viewer

Your web browser doesn't have a PDF plugin. Please download the file


Full Text: PDF | SPANISH TRANSLATION |





The texts published in this journal, unless otherwise indicated, are subject to a Creative Commons Attribution-Noncommercial-NoDerivativeWorks 3.0 Spain license. They may be copied, distributed and broadcasted provided that the author, the journal (NAER, New Approaches in Educational Research), and the institution that publishes them are cited. Commercial use and derivative works are not permitted. The full license can be consulted on http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/3.0/es/deed.en.


N.Appr.Ed.Rs. e-ISSN 2254-7339. DOI 10.7821 NAER. Rosabel Roig Vila. University of Alicante. Carretera San Vicente del Raspeig s/n - 03690 San Vicente del Raspeig - Alicante - Spain. Phone:(+34) 96 590 37 21 | Fax: (+34) 96 590 3721 | editor.naerjournal@ua.es | http://naerjournal.ua.es Developed with Open Journal System